- home
- Advanced Search
- Energy Research
- Energy Research
description Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2023 Netherlands, United Kingdom, Czech Republic, Netherlands, Czech Republic, Netherlands, AustriaPublisher:Springer Science and Business Media LLC Funded by:NSF | DEVIATE: Using positive d...NSF| DEVIATE: Using positive deviance to reduce economic and health inequalityBojana Većkalov; Sandra J. Geiger; František Bartoš; Mathew P. White; Bastiaan T. Rutjens; Frenk van Harreveld; Federica Stablum; Berkan Akın; Alaa Aldoh; Jinhao Bai; Frida Berglund; Aleša Bratina Zimic; Margaret Broyles; Andrea Catania; Airu Chen; Magdalena Chorzępa; Eman Farahat; Jakob Götz; Bat Hoter-Ishay; Gesine Jordan; Siri Joustra; Jonas Klingebiel; Živa Krajnc; Antonia Krug; Thomas Lind Andersen; Johanna Löloff; Divya Natarajan; Sasha Newman-Oktan; Elena Niehoff; Celeste Paerels; Rachel Papirmeister; Steven Peregrina; Felicia Pohl; Amanda Remsö; Abigail Roh; Binahayati Rusyidi; Justus Schmidt; Mariam Shavgulidze; Valentina Vellinho Nardin; Ruixiang Wang; Kelly Warner; Miranda Wattier; Chloe Y. Wong; Mariem Younssi; Kai Ruggeri; Sander van der Linden;pmid: 39187712
pmc: PMC11493676
AbstractCommunicating the scientific consensus that human-caused climate change is real increases climate change beliefs, worry and support for public action in the United States. In this preregistered experiment, we tested two scientific consensus messages, a classic message on the reality of human-caused climate change and an updated message additionally emphasizing scientific agreement that climate change is a crisis. Across online convenience samples from 27 countries (n = 10,527), the classic message substantially reduces misperceptions (d = 0.47, 95% CI (0.41, 0.52)) and slightly increases climate change beliefs (from d = 0.06, 95% CI (0.01, 0.11) to d = 0.10, 95% CI (0.04, 0.15)) and worry (d = 0.05, 95% CI (−0.01, 0.10)) but not support for public action directly. The updated message is equally effective but provides no added value. Both messages are more effective for audiences with lower message familiarity and higher misperceptions, including those with lower trust in climate scientists and right-leaning ideologies. Overall, scientific consensus messaging is an effective, non-polarizing tool for changing misperceptions, beliefs and worry across different audiences.
Nature Human Behavio... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRepository of the Czech Academy of SciencesArticle . 2024Data sources: Repository of the Czech Academy of SciencesDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2024 . Peer-reviewedWeb-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsArticle . 2024Data sources: Web-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsNature Human BehaviourArticle . 2024add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41562-024-01928-2&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eu10 citations 10 popularity Average influence Average impulse Top 10% Powered by BIP!
more_vert Nature Human Behavio... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRepository of the Czech Academy of SciencesArticle . 2024Data sources: Repository of the Czech Academy of SciencesDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2024 . Peer-reviewedWeb-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsArticle . 2024Data sources: Web-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsNature Human BehaviourArticle . 2024add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41562-024-01928-2&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2025Embargo end date: 20 Jan 2025 Italy, Sweden, Serbia, United Kingdom, United Kingdom, Netherlands, Serbia, Belgium, Switzerland, Switzerland, United Kingdom, Austria, AustriaPublisher:Springer Science and Business Media LLC Publicly fundedFunded by:EC | JITSUVAX, SNSF | Scientific and public per..., WT +8 projectsEC| JITSUVAX ,SNSF| Scientific and public perceptions of the political terrain of climate change science ,WT ,SSHRC ,ANR| PSL ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP190101675 ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP180102384 ,EC| Governance ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP190101507 ,ARC| ARC Future Fellowships - Grant ID: FT190100708 ,EC| CONSPIRACY_FXNiels G. Mede; Viktoria Cologna; Sebastian Berger; John Besley; Cameron Brick; Marina Joubert; Edward W. Maibach; Sabina Mihelj; Naomi Oreskes; Mike S. Schäfer; Sander van der Linden; Nor Izzatina Abdul Aziz; Suleiman Abdulsalam; Nurulaini Abu Shamsi; Balazs Aczel; Indro Adinugroho; Eleonora Alabrese; Alaa Aldoh; Mark Alfano; Innocent Mbulli Ali; Mohammed Alsobay; Marlene Altenmüller; R. Michael Alvarez; Richard Amoako; Tabitha Amollo; Patrick Ansah; Denisa Apriliawati; Flavio Azevedo; Ani Bajrami; Ronita Bardhan; Keagile Bati; Eri Bertsou; Cornelia Betsch; Apurav Yash Bhatiya; Rahul Bhui; Olga Białobrzeska; Michał Bilewicz; Ayoub Bouguettaya; Katherine Breeden; Amélie Bret; Ondrej Buchel; Pablo Cabrera-Álvarez; Federica Cagnoli; André Calero Valdez; Timothy Callaghan; Rizza Kaye Cases; Sami Çoksan; Gabriela Czarnek; Steven De Peuter; Ramit Debnath; Sylvain Delouvée; Lucia Di Stefano; Celia Díaz-Catalán; Kimberly C. Doell; Simone Dohle; Karen M. Douglas; Charlotte Dries; Dmitrii Dubrov; Małgorzata Dzimińska; Ullrich K. H. Ecker; Christian T. Elbaek; Mahmoud Elsherif; Benjamin Enke; Tom W. Etienne; Matthew Facciani; Antoinette Fage-Butler; Md. Zaki Faisal; Xiaoli Fan; Christina Farhart; Christoph Feldhaus; Marinus Ferreira; Stefan Feuerriegel; Helen Fischer; Jana Freundt; Malte Friese; Simon Fuglsang; Albina Gallyamova; Patricia Garrido-Vásquez; Mauricio E. Garrido Vásquez; Winfred Gatua; Oliver Genschow; Omid Ghasemi; Theofilos Gkinopoulos; Jamie L. Gloor; Ellen Goddard; Mario Gollwitzer; Claudia González-Brambila; Hazel Gordon; Dmitry Grigoryev; Gina M. Grimshaw; Lars Guenther; Håvard Haarstad; Dana Harari; Lelia N. Hawkins; Przemysław Hensel; Alma Cristal Hernández-Mondragón; Atar Herziger; Guanxiong Huang; Markus Huff; Mairéad Hurley; Nygmet Ibadildin; Maho Ishibashi; Mohammad Tarikul Islam; Younes Jeddi; Tao Jin; Charlotte A. Jones; Sebastian Jungkunz; Dominika Jurgiel; Zhangir Kabdulkair; Jo-Ju Kao; Sarah Kavassalis; John R. Kerr; Mariana Kitsa; Tereza Klabíková Rábová; Olivier Klein; Hoyoun Koh; Aki Koivula; Lilian Kojan; Elizaveta Komyaginskaya; Laura König; Lina Koppel; Kochav Koren Nobre Cavalcante; Alexandra Kosachenko; John Kotcher; Laura S. Kranz; Pradeep Krishnan; Silje Kristiansen; André Krouwel; Toon Kuppens; Eleni A. Kyza; Claus Lamm; Anthony Lantian; Aleksandra Lazić; Oscar Lecuona; Jean-Baptiste Légal; Zoe Leviston; Neil Levy; Amanda M. Lindkvist; Grégoire Lits; Andreas Löschel; Alberto López-Ortega; Carlos Lopez-Villavicencio; Nigel Mantou Lou; Chloe H. Lucas; Kristin Lunz-Trujillo; Mathew D. Marques; Sabrina J. Mayer; Ryan McKay; Hugo Mercier; Julia Metag; Taciano L. Milfont; Joanne M. Miller; Panagiotis Mitkidis; Fredy Monge-Rodríguez; Matt Motta; Iryna Mudra; Zarja Muršič; Jennifer Namutebi; Eryn J. Newman; Jonas P. Nitschke; Ntui-Njock Vincent Ntui; Daniel Nwogwugwu; Thomas Ostermann; Tobias Otterbring; Jaime Palmer-Hague; Myrto Pantazi; Philip Pärnamets; Paolo Parra Saiani; Mariola Paruzel-Czachura; Michal Parzuchowski; Yuri G. Pavlov; Adam R. Pearson; Myron A. Penner; Charlotte R. Pennington; Katerina Petkanopoulou; Marija M. Petrović; Jan Pfänder; Dinara Pisareva; Adam Ploszaj; Karolína Poliaková; Ekaterina Pronizius; Katarzyna Pypno-Blajda; Diwa Malaya A. Quiñones; Pekka Räsänen; Adrian Rauchfleisch; Felix G. Rebitschek; Cintia Refojo Seronero; Gabriel Rêgo; James P. Reynolds; Joseph Roche; Simone Rödder; Jan Philipp Röer; Robert M. Ross; Isabelle Ruin; Osvaldo Santos; Ricardo R. Santos; Philipp Schmid; Stefan Schulreich; Bermond Scoggins; Amena Sharaf;pmid: 39833242
pmc: PMC11747281
Abstract Science is integral to society because it can inform individual, government, corporate, and civil society decision-making on issues such as public health, new technologies or climate change. Yet, public distrust and populist sentiment challenge the relationship between science and society. To help researchers analyse the science-society nexus across different geographical and cultural contexts, we undertook a cross-sectional population survey resulting in a dataset of 71,922 participants in 68 countries. The data were collected between November 2022 and August 2023 as part of the global Many Labs study “Trust in Science and Science-Related Populism” (TISP). The questionnaire contained comprehensive measures for individuals’ trust in scientists, science-related populist attitudes, perceptions of the role of science in society, science media use and communication behaviour, attitudes to climate change and support for environmental policies, personality traits, political and religious views and demographic characteristics. Here, we describe the dataset, survey materials and psychometric properties of key variables. We encourage researchers to use this unique dataset for global comparative analyses on public perceptions of science and its role in society and policy-making.
Scientific Data arrow_drop_down REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyOxford University Research ArchiveArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: Oxford University Research ArchiveVrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalArticle . 2025Data sources: Vrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalPublikationer från Linköpings universitetArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedData sources: Publikationer från Linköpings universitetDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedUniversity of Bristol: Bristol ResearchArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)University of St. Gallen: DSpaceArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41597-024-04100-7&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen hybrid 3 citations 3 popularity Average influence Average impulse Average Powered by BIP!
more_vert Scientific Data arrow_drop_down REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyOxford University Research ArchiveArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: Oxford University Research ArchiveVrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalArticle . 2025Data sources: Vrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalPublikationer från Linköpings universitetArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedData sources: Publikationer från Linköpings universitetDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedUniversity of Bristol: Bristol ResearchArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)University of St. Gallen: DSpaceArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41597-024-04100-7&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Journal , Other literature type 2021Publisher:MDPI AG Authors: Alaa Aldoh; Paul Sparks; Peter R. Harris;doi: 10.3390/su13158315
Recent research in the US shows the potential of providing information about minority norms that are increasing on positively influencing interest and engagement in desired behaviours. Although these are promising findings, there is little published research replicating or testing this effect outside the US. The study reported here is a direct replication of Sparkman and Walton’s (2017) research. We explored the effects of different kinds of normative information, particularly information about increasing (referred to as ‘dynamic’ or ‘trending’) minority norms, on interest in reducing meat consumption, attitudes toward reducing meat consumption, intentions to reduce meat consumption, and expectations to do so. Following pilot work (n = 197), we used a double-blind online study with three conditions: dynamic norm (n = 276), static norm (n = 284), and no norm (n = 286). The sample consisted of British people, with ages ranging from 18 to 79 (Mage = 37.21, SDage = 13.58; 56.38% female). There was no effect of dynamic norm information on any outcomes, including predictions about future meat consumption norms. Exploratory analyses suggest that political position and gender were associated with meat consumption outcomes. The findings are discussed in relation to conditions under which dynamic normative information may be successful in influencing motivation to engage in desired behaviours, and to possible improvements in research design.
Sustainability arrow_drop_down SustainabilityOther literature type . 2021License: CC BYFull-Text: http://www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/13/15/8315/pdfData sources: Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Instituteadd ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.3390/su13158315&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen gold 25 citations 25 popularity Top 10% influence Average impulse Top 10% Powered by BIP!
more_vert Sustainability arrow_drop_down SustainabilityOther literature type . 2021License: CC BYFull-Text: http://www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/13/15/8315/pdfData sources: Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Instituteadd ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.3390/su13158315&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2023 NetherlandsPublisher:Center for Open Science Authors: Alaa Aldoh; Paul Sparks; Peter R. Harris;pmid: 39100183
pmc: PMC11296159
While decreasing their meat consumption is one of the most impactful behaviours an individual may carry out to reduce their carbon emissions, it is still a minority behaviour in many parts of the world. Research suggests that communicating information about changing ‘dynamic’ norms may be a useful tool for changing attitudes and behaviours in the direction of those currently held by the minority. This study utilizes a 2 x 2 mixed design (Norm Type [dynamic/static] x Visual Cue [present/absent], and a no-task control), and a follow-up assessment after one week to investigate the effect of making dynamic norms salient on various meat consumption outcomes: attitudes towards meat consumption, interest in reducing one’s own meat consumption, intentions to reduce one’s own meat consumption, and self-reported meat consumption. We used an online sample of British participants (N = 1294), ranging in ages 18–77 (Mage = 39.97, SDage = 13.71; 55.8% female). We hypothesized that: (a) dynamic norms will positively influence meat consumption outcomes, (b) visual cues will accentuate the difference between norm conditions, (c) using a visual cue will enhance the effect of dynamic norms, and (d) any effects of dynamic norms will endure over a period of one week. We found no positive effect of dynamic norms (vs. static norms) on any outcome at time 1, and no positive effect on changes in outcomes from time 1 to time 2. However, we found a positive interaction of norm type and visual cue at time 1 (although not from time 1 to time 2): the addition of a visual cue to dynamic norm messages enhanced the positive effect of the message at time 1 (but did not enhance the changes occurring from time 1 to time 2). Analyses for changes in self-reported meat consumption did not reach our evidential threshold. We discuss the practical and theoretical implications of these findings.
https://doi.org/10.3... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRoyal Society Open ScienceArticle . 2024License: CC BYData sources: Universiteit van Amsterdam Digital Academic RepositoryUniversiteit van Amsterdam: Digital Academic Repository (UvA DARE)Article . 2024Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.31234/osf.io/tac3j&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen gold 1 citations 1 popularity Top 10% influence Top 10% impulse Average Powered by BIP!
more_vert https://doi.org/10.3... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRoyal Society Open ScienceArticle . 2024License: CC BYData sources: Universiteit van Amsterdam Digital Academic RepositoryUniversiteit van Amsterdam: Digital Academic Repository (UvA DARE)Article . 2024Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.31234/osf.io/tac3j&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eu
description Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2023 Netherlands, United Kingdom, Czech Republic, Netherlands, Czech Republic, Netherlands, AustriaPublisher:Springer Science and Business Media LLC Funded by:NSF | DEVIATE: Using positive d...NSF| DEVIATE: Using positive deviance to reduce economic and health inequalityBojana Većkalov; Sandra J. Geiger; František Bartoš; Mathew P. White; Bastiaan T. Rutjens; Frenk van Harreveld; Federica Stablum; Berkan Akın; Alaa Aldoh; Jinhao Bai; Frida Berglund; Aleša Bratina Zimic; Margaret Broyles; Andrea Catania; Airu Chen; Magdalena Chorzępa; Eman Farahat; Jakob Götz; Bat Hoter-Ishay; Gesine Jordan; Siri Joustra; Jonas Klingebiel; Živa Krajnc; Antonia Krug; Thomas Lind Andersen; Johanna Löloff; Divya Natarajan; Sasha Newman-Oktan; Elena Niehoff; Celeste Paerels; Rachel Papirmeister; Steven Peregrina; Felicia Pohl; Amanda Remsö; Abigail Roh; Binahayati Rusyidi; Justus Schmidt; Mariam Shavgulidze; Valentina Vellinho Nardin; Ruixiang Wang; Kelly Warner; Miranda Wattier; Chloe Y. Wong; Mariem Younssi; Kai Ruggeri; Sander van der Linden;pmid: 39187712
pmc: PMC11493676
AbstractCommunicating the scientific consensus that human-caused climate change is real increases climate change beliefs, worry and support for public action in the United States. In this preregistered experiment, we tested two scientific consensus messages, a classic message on the reality of human-caused climate change and an updated message additionally emphasizing scientific agreement that climate change is a crisis. Across online convenience samples from 27 countries (n = 10,527), the classic message substantially reduces misperceptions (d = 0.47, 95% CI (0.41, 0.52)) and slightly increases climate change beliefs (from d = 0.06, 95% CI (0.01, 0.11) to d = 0.10, 95% CI (0.04, 0.15)) and worry (d = 0.05, 95% CI (−0.01, 0.10)) but not support for public action directly. The updated message is equally effective but provides no added value. Both messages are more effective for audiences with lower message familiarity and higher misperceptions, including those with lower trust in climate scientists and right-leaning ideologies. Overall, scientific consensus messaging is an effective, non-polarizing tool for changing misperceptions, beliefs and worry across different audiences.
Nature Human Behavio... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRepository of the Czech Academy of SciencesArticle . 2024Data sources: Repository of the Czech Academy of SciencesDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2024 . Peer-reviewedWeb-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsArticle . 2024Data sources: Web-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsNature Human BehaviourArticle . 2024add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41562-024-01928-2&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eu10 citations 10 popularity Average influence Average impulse Top 10% Powered by BIP!
more_vert Nature Human Behavio... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRepository of the Czech Academy of SciencesArticle . 2024Data sources: Repository of the Czech Academy of SciencesDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2024 . Peer-reviewedWeb-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsArticle . 2024Data sources: Web-based Archive of RIVM PublicationsNature Human BehaviourArticle . 2024add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41562-024-01928-2&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2025Embargo end date: 20 Jan 2025 Italy, Sweden, Serbia, United Kingdom, United Kingdom, Netherlands, Serbia, Belgium, Switzerland, Switzerland, United Kingdom, Austria, AustriaPublisher:Springer Science and Business Media LLC Publicly fundedFunded by:EC | JITSUVAX, SNSF | Scientific and public per..., WT +8 projectsEC| JITSUVAX ,SNSF| Scientific and public perceptions of the political terrain of climate change science ,WT ,SSHRC ,ANR| PSL ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP190101675 ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP180102384 ,EC| Governance ,ARC| Discovery Projects - Grant ID: DP190101507 ,ARC| ARC Future Fellowships - Grant ID: FT190100708 ,EC| CONSPIRACY_FXNiels G. Mede; Viktoria Cologna; Sebastian Berger; John Besley; Cameron Brick; Marina Joubert; Edward W. Maibach; Sabina Mihelj; Naomi Oreskes; Mike S. Schäfer; Sander van der Linden; Nor Izzatina Abdul Aziz; Suleiman Abdulsalam; Nurulaini Abu Shamsi; Balazs Aczel; Indro Adinugroho; Eleonora Alabrese; Alaa Aldoh; Mark Alfano; Innocent Mbulli Ali; Mohammed Alsobay; Marlene Altenmüller; R. Michael Alvarez; Richard Amoako; Tabitha Amollo; Patrick Ansah; Denisa Apriliawati; Flavio Azevedo; Ani Bajrami; Ronita Bardhan; Keagile Bati; Eri Bertsou; Cornelia Betsch; Apurav Yash Bhatiya; Rahul Bhui; Olga Białobrzeska; Michał Bilewicz; Ayoub Bouguettaya; Katherine Breeden; Amélie Bret; Ondrej Buchel; Pablo Cabrera-Álvarez; Federica Cagnoli; André Calero Valdez; Timothy Callaghan; Rizza Kaye Cases; Sami Çoksan; Gabriela Czarnek; Steven De Peuter; Ramit Debnath; Sylvain Delouvée; Lucia Di Stefano; Celia Díaz-Catalán; Kimberly C. Doell; Simone Dohle; Karen M. Douglas; Charlotte Dries; Dmitrii Dubrov; Małgorzata Dzimińska; Ullrich K. H. Ecker; Christian T. Elbaek; Mahmoud Elsherif; Benjamin Enke; Tom W. Etienne; Matthew Facciani; Antoinette Fage-Butler; Md. Zaki Faisal; Xiaoli Fan; Christina Farhart; Christoph Feldhaus; Marinus Ferreira; Stefan Feuerriegel; Helen Fischer; Jana Freundt; Malte Friese; Simon Fuglsang; Albina Gallyamova; Patricia Garrido-Vásquez; Mauricio E. Garrido Vásquez; Winfred Gatua; Oliver Genschow; Omid Ghasemi; Theofilos Gkinopoulos; Jamie L. Gloor; Ellen Goddard; Mario Gollwitzer; Claudia González-Brambila; Hazel Gordon; Dmitry Grigoryev; Gina M. Grimshaw; Lars Guenther; Håvard Haarstad; Dana Harari; Lelia N. Hawkins; Przemysław Hensel; Alma Cristal Hernández-Mondragón; Atar Herziger; Guanxiong Huang; Markus Huff; Mairéad Hurley; Nygmet Ibadildin; Maho Ishibashi; Mohammad Tarikul Islam; Younes Jeddi; Tao Jin; Charlotte A. Jones; Sebastian Jungkunz; Dominika Jurgiel; Zhangir Kabdulkair; Jo-Ju Kao; Sarah Kavassalis; John R. Kerr; Mariana Kitsa; Tereza Klabíková Rábová; Olivier Klein; Hoyoun Koh; Aki Koivula; Lilian Kojan; Elizaveta Komyaginskaya; Laura König; Lina Koppel; Kochav Koren Nobre Cavalcante; Alexandra Kosachenko; John Kotcher; Laura S. Kranz; Pradeep Krishnan; Silje Kristiansen; André Krouwel; Toon Kuppens; Eleni A. Kyza; Claus Lamm; Anthony Lantian; Aleksandra Lazić; Oscar Lecuona; Jean-Baptiste Légal; Zoe Leviston; Neil Levy; Amanda M. Lindkvist; Grégoire Lits; Andreas Löschel; Alberto López-Ortega; Carlos Lopez-Villavicencio; Nigel Mantou Lou; Chloe H. Lucas; Kristin Lunz-Trujillo; Mathew D. Marques; Sabrina J. Mayer; Ryan McKay; Hugo Mercier; Julia Metag; Taciano L. Milfont; Joanne M. Miller; Panagiotis Mitkidis; Fredy Monge-Rodríguez; Matt Motta; Iryna Mudra; Zarja Muršič; Jennifer Namutebi; Eryn J. Newman; Jonas P. Nitschke; Ntui-Njock Vincent Ntui; Daniel Nwogwugwu; Thomas Ostermann; Tobias Otterbring; Jaime Palmer-Hague; Myrto Pantazi; Philip Pärnamets; Paolo Parra Saiani; Mariola Paruzel-Czachura; Michal Parzuchowski; Yuri G. Pavlov; Adam R. Pearson; Myron A. Penner; Charlotte R. Pennington; Katerina Petkanopoulou; Marija M. Petrović; Jan Pfänder; Dinara Pisareva; Adam Ploszaj; Karolína Poliaková; Ekaterina Pronizius; Katarzyna Pypno-Blajda; Diwa Malaya A. Quiñones; Pekka Räsänen; Adrian Rauchfleisch; Felix G. Rebitschek; Cintia Refojo Seronero; Gabriel Rêgo; James P. Reynolds; Joseph Roche; Simone Rödder; Jan Philipp Röer; Robert M. Ross; Isabelle Ruin; Osvaldo Santos; Ricardo R. Santos; Philipp Schmid; Stefan Schulreich; Bermond Scoggins; Amena Sharaf;pmid: 39833242
pmc: PMC11747281
Abstract Science is integral to society because it can inform individual, government, corporate, and civil society decision-making on issues such as public health, new technologies or climate change. Yet, public distrust and populist sentiment challenge the relationship between science and society. To help researchers analyse the science-society nexus across different geographical and cultural contexts, we undertook a cross-sectional population survey resulting in a dataset of 71,922 participants in 68 countries. The data were collected between November 2022 and August 2023 as part of the global Many Labs study “Trust in Science and Science-Related Populism” (TISP). The questionnaire contained comprehensive measures for individuals’ trust in scientists, science-related populist attitudes, perceptions of the role of science in society, science media use and communication behaviour, attitudes to climate change and support for environmental policies, personality traits, political and religious views and demographic characteristics. Here, we describe the dataset, survey materials and psychometric properties of key variables. We encourage researchers to use this unique dataset for global comparative analyses on public perceptions of science and its role in society and policy-making.
Scientific Data arrow_drop_down REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyOxford University Research ArchiveArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: Oxford University Research ArchiveVrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalArticle . 2025Data sources: Vrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalPublikationer från Linköpings universitetArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedData sources: Publikationer från Linköpings universitetDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedUniversity of Bristol: Bristol ResearchArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)University of St. Gallen: DSpaceArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41597-024-04100-7&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen hybrid 3 citations 3 popularity Average influence Average impulse Average Powered by BIP!
more_vert Scientific Data arrow_drop_down REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: REFF - University of Belgrade - Faculty of PhilosophyOxford University Research ArchiveArticle . 2025License: CC BYData sources: Oxford University Research ArchiveVrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalArticle . 2025Data sources: Vrije Universiteit Brussel Research PortalPublikationer från Linköpings universitetArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedData sources: Publikationer från Linköpings universitetDigitala Vetenskapliga Arkivet - Academic Archive On-lineArticle . 2025 . Peer-reviewedUniversity of Bristol: Bristol ResearchArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)University of St. Gallen: DSpaceArticle . 2025Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.1038/s41597-024-04100-7&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Journal , Other literature type 2021Publisher:MDPI AG Authors: Alaa Aldoh; Paul Sparks; Peter R. Harris;doi: 10.3390/su13158315
Recent research in the US shows the potential of providing information about minority norms that are increasing on positively influencing interest and engagement in desired behaviours. Although these are promising findings, there is little published research replicating or testing this effect outside the US. The study reported here is a direct replication of Sparkman and Walton’s (2017) research. We explored the effects of different kinds of normative information, particularly information about increasing (referred to as ‘dynamic’ or ‘trending’) minority norms, on interest in reducing meat consumption, attitudes toward reducing meat consumption, intentions to reduce meat consumption, and expectations to do so. Following pilot work (n = 197), we used a double-blind online study with three conditions: dynamic norm (n = 276), static norm (n = 284), and no norm (n = 286). The sample consisted of British people, with ages ranging from 18 to 79 (Mage = 37.21, SDage = 13.58; 56.38% female). There was no effect of dynamic norm information on any outcomes, including predictions about future meat consumption norms. Exploratory analyses suggest that political position and gender were associated with meat consumption outcomes. The findings are discussed in relation to conditions under which dynamic normative information may be successful in influencing motivation to engage in desired behaviours, and to possible improvements in research design.
Sustainability arrow_drop_down SustainabilityOther literature type . 2021License: CC BYFull-Text: http://www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/13/15/8315/pdfData sources: Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Instituteadd ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.3390/su13158315&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen gold 25 citations 25 popularity Top 10% influence Average impulse Top 10% Powered by BIP!
more_vert Sustainability arrow_drop_down SustainabilityOther literature type . 2021License: CC BYFull-Text: http://www.mdpi.com/2071-1050/13/15/8315/pdfData sources: Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Instituteadd ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.3390/su13158315&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eudescription Publicationkeyboard_double_arrow_right Article , Other literature type 2023 NetherlandsPublisher:Center for Open Science Authors: Alaa Aldoh; Paul Sparks; Peter R. Harris;pmid: 39100183
pmc: PMC11296159
While decreasing their meat consumption is one of the most impactful behaviours an individual may carry out to reduce their carbon emissions, it is still a minority behaviour in many parts of the world. Research suggests that communicating information about changing ‘dynamic’ norms may be a useful tool for changing attitudes and behaviours in the direction of those currently held by the minority. This study utilizes a 2 x 2 mixed design (Norm Type [dynamic/static] x Visual Cue [present/absent], and a no-task control), and a follow-up assessment after one week to investigate the effect of making dynamic norms salient on various meat consumption outcomes: attitudes towards meat consumption, interest in reducing one’s own meat consumption, intentions to reduce one’s own meat consumption, and self-reported meat consumption. We used an online sample of British participants (N = 1294), ranging in ages 18–77 (Mage = 39.97, SDage = 13.71; 55.8% female). We hypothesized that: (a) dynamic norms will positively influence meat consumption outcomes, (b) visual cues will accentuate the difference between norm conditions, (c) using a visual cue will enhance the effect of dynamic norms, and (d) any effects of dynamic norms will endure over a period of one week. We found no positive effect of dynamic norms (vs. static norms) on any outcome at time 1, and no positive effect on changes in outcomes from time 1 to time 2. However, we found a positive interaction of norm type and visual cue at time 1 (although not from time 1 to time 2): the addition of a visual cue to dynamic norm messages enhanced the positive effect of the message at time 1 (but did not enhance the changes occurring from time 1 to time 2). Analyses for changes in self-reported meat consumption did not reach our evidential threshold. We discuss the practical and theoretical implications of these findings.
https://doi.org/10.3... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRoyal Society Open ScienceArticle . 2024License: CC BYData sources: Universiteit van Amsterdam Digital Academic RepositoryUniversiteit van Amsterdam: Digital Academic Repository (UvA DARE)Article . 2024Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.31234/osf.io/tac3j&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.euAccess RoutesGreen gold 1 citations 1 popularity Top 10% influence Top 10% impulse Average Powered by BIP!
more_vert https://doi.org/10.3... arrow_drop_down https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.i...Article . 2023 . Peer-reviewedLicense: CC BYData sources: CrossrefRoyal Society Open ScienceArticle . 2024License: CC BYData sources: Universiteit van Amsterdam Digital Academic RepositoryUniversiteit van Amsterdam: Digital Academic Repository (UvA DARE)Article . 2024Data sources: Bielefeld Academic Search Engine (BASE)add ClaimPlease grant OpenAIRE to access and update your ORCID works.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.This Research product is the result of merged Research products in OpenAIRE.
You have already added works in your ORCID record related to the merged Research product.All Research productsarrow_drop_down <script type="text/javascript"> <!-- document.write('<div id="oa_widget"></div>'); document.write('<script type="text/javascript" src="https://beta.openaire.eu/index.php?option=com_openaire&view=widget&format=raw&projectId=10.31234/osf.io/tac3j&type=result"></script>'); --> </script>
For further information contact us at helpdesk@openaire.eu